Skip to content

Commit

Permalink
Final edits before resubmission
Browse files Browse the repository at this point in the history
  • Loading branch information
lgs85 committed Jul 13, 2017
1 parent c54f313 commit 182c06c
Show file tree
Hide file tree
Showing 9 changed files with 13 additions and 13 deletions.
Binary file modified .DS_Store
Binary file not shown.
16 changes: 8 additions & 8 deletions Manuscript/Response_to_reviewers_2.Rmd
Original file line number Diff line number Diff line change
@@ -1,6 +1,6 @@
---
output:
word_document:
pdf_document:
header-includes:
\usepackage{graphicx}
\usepackage{changepage}
Expand Down Expand Up @@ -70,7 +70,7 @@ Lewis Spurgin (on behalf of all authors)

**One of the original reviewers and myself have now read your revised manuscript and we both think that it has improved substantially. The reviewer, however, very nicely describes some points that I strongly agree would need more attention in the discussion of the paper to more honestly balance/describe the strong AND weak points of your study. These weaker points do not decrease the impact, novelty or relevance of your study, but discussing them in more detail could rather inspire advances to the methodology used in your field of research.**

RESPONSE: We are glad that you both agree that our manuscript has improved, and thank you again for your constructive comments. We agree with the central point made by reviewer and yourself, and have now adjusted our discussion accordingly. We hope you agree that this has further improved our manuscript, and we are hopful that this improved version will generate discussion about both the ecological and technical aspects of telomere dynamics in wild populations.
RESPONSE: We are glad that you both agree that our manuscript has improved, and thank you again for your constructive comments. We agree with the central point made by reviewer and yourself, and have now adjusted our discussion accordingly. We hope you agree that this has further improved our manuscript, and we are hopeful that this improved version will generate discussion about both the ecological and technical aspects of telomere dynamics in wild populations.

\newpage

Expand All @@ -86,7 +86,7 @@ RESPONSE: We are glad that you both agree that our manuscript has improved, and

**Line 441: It is very kind of the authors to acknowledge me by name, but my first name is Antoine, not Eric...**

RESPONSE: We are very sorry about this, and we do of course know that your name is Antoine! We have now changed the ackowledgements.
RESPONSE: We are very sorry about this, and we do of course know that your name is Antoine! We have now changed the acknowledgements.

\bigskip

Expand All @@ -107,7 +107,7 @@ $variation between groups / (variation between groups + variation within groups)

Thus if within-group variation is extremely small (i.e. no measurement error), the ICC would be 1, so 0% of the variation would be explained by measurement error. We would therefore interpret an ICC of 0.68 as 68% of the variation being explained by sample identity and 32% by variation other than sample identity (i.e. measurement error).

However, this is a debate over semantics. In the manuscript, we are careful to phrase things in terms of repeatability and avoid using pearson R and R2 values as these are generally considered inappropriate for measuring repeatability. Nowhere do we state that 68% of the variance can be explained by sample identity.
However, this is a debate over semantics. In the manuscript, we are careful to phrase things in terms of repeatability and avoid using Pearson R and R2 values as these are generally considered inappropriate for measuring repeatability. Nowhere do we state that 68% of the variance can be explained by sample identity.

\bigskip

Expand All @@ -124,12 +124,12 @@ However, this is a debate over semantics. In the manuscript, we are careful to p

**qPCR efficiency: An efficiency of 78% means that the qPCR conditions have not been optimized properly. Correcting with LinReg for differences in efficiency is minimizing noise related to such problem, but not fixing the problem when efficiency is so different from 100%. Again, this is likely to increase measurement error, but not to create false positive. Such limitation should be discussed in the paper too. The fact that others are publishing with poor efficiency is not a good answer in my opinion, and I can cite you many papers reporting efficiencies being in the range 90-110%, including most (all?) papers of Criscuolo’s lab and Monaghan’s lab. In LinReg, are you correcting for sample specific efficiency or per the average plate efficiency? Correcting for sample specific efficiency has been criticized in the past : "Sample specific PCR efficiency estimation has its usefulness, but currently only for outlier detection", Hellemans et al. 2007 qBase relative quantification framework and software for management and automated analysis of real-time quantitative PCR data.**

RESPONSE: We are glad that the reviewer agrees that our observed repeatability/efficiency values will not lead to false positives. Our dataset includes repeats that have run as much as six years apart, and these repeats are included in our repeatability estimates. We suspect that our dataset is fairly unique in this respect at present, but other studies will certainly have such data available in the near future. The Bebbington et al. paper only included a set of more recently run telomere samples, hence the higher repeatability. In this paper, the aim was to cpature variation (and repeatability) among as money cohorts and samples as possible. We agree that this issue deserves more discussion space, and have now devoted an entire paragraph to the issue of repeatability, and the specific issue of qPCR efficiency, in the discussion on lines 414-428:
RESPONSE: We are glad that the reviewer agrees that our observed repeatability/efficiency values will not lead to false positives. Our dataset includes repeats that have run as much as six years apart, and these repeats are included in our repeatability estimates. We suspect that our dataset is fairly unique in this respect at present, but other studies will certainly have such data available in the near future. The Bebbington et al. paper only included a set of more recently run telomere samples, hence the higher repeatability. In this paper, the aim was to capture variation (and repeatability) among as money cohorts and samples as possible. We agree that this issue deserves more discussion space, and have now devoted an entire paragraph to the issue of repeatability, and the specific issue of qPCR efficiency, in the discussion on lines 414-428:

\begin{adjustwidth}{2cm}{2cm}

\textit{
It is worth considering the fact that the social and ecological variables we tested here explained only a small proportion of the variance in RTL. Furthermore, some factors such as territory quality and social group size were not related to telomere dynamics when we may have expected them to be (Van de Crommenacker et al. 2011; Brouwer, Richardson, and Komdeur 2012). Measurement error is clearly an issue in our study, and has almost certainly decreased the explanatory power of our models, and elevated levels of Type II error. The low repeatability we observed is the product of i) lower levels of efficiency in the telomere qPCR reaction than we would have liked, ii) the long-term nature of the study. Samples for this study were run aver a period of several years, during which reagents, consumables and personnel all change. Our repeatability estimate includes samples that were run several years apart, and reflect all the sources of error that accumulated over that time. Compared to a set-up where a small amount of samples are all run at the same time, it is unsurprising that we have a higher error rate. Techniques for measuring telomere length with a greater degree of precision are likely to prove helpful in future ecological studies of telomere dynamics (Nussey et al. 2014), and discussions are now required on how to best optimise measuring telomere length for long-term studies. A central issue to resolve is how best to balance the trade-off between obtaining precise telomere measurements, and utilising the large sample sizes necessary for ecological study.
It is worth considering the fact that the social and ecological variables we tested here explained only a small proportion of the variance in RTL. Furthermore, some factors such as territory quality and social group size were not related to telomere dynamics when we may have expected them to be (Van de Crommenacker et al. 2011; Brouwer, Richardson, and Komdeur 2012). Measurement error is clearly an issue in our study, and has almost certainly decreased the explanatory power of our models, and elevated levels of Type II error. The low repeatability we observed is the product of i) lower levels of efficiency in the telomere qPCR reaction than we would have liked, ii) the long-term nature of the study. Samples for this study were run over a period of up to six years, during which reagents, consumables and personnel all change. Our repeatability estimate includes samples that were run several years apart, and reflect all the sources of error that accumulated over that time. Compared to a set-up where a small amount of samples are all run at the same time, it is unsurprising that we have a higher error rate - indeed, in our own studies where we have run all the samples within a short timeframe we have observed higher repeatability (Barrett et al. 2012; Bebbington et al. 2016). Techniques for measuring telomere length with a greater degree of precision are likely to prove helpful in future long-term ecological studies of telomere dynamics (Nussey et al. 2014), and discussions are now required on how to best optimise measuring telomere length for long-term studies. A central issue to resolve is how best to balance the trade-off between obtaining precise telomere measurements, and utilising the large sample sizes necessary for ecological study.
}

\end{adjustwidth}
Expand Down Expand Up @@ -177,12 +177,12 @@ Our study adds to the substantial body of literature from humans and wild animal
**Do you have any idea about the presence of interstitial telomeric sequence in your study species? Because presence of ITS would even artificially increase intra-individual repeatability when telomeres are measured with qPCR.
If there is only 8% of intra-individual repeatability, it is quite surprising that telomere length at a given point can predict survival, as shown by many studies including in your own study system. With such a low intra-individual repeatability, I agree that telomeres can be seen as a biomarker of short-term “costs”, but we would have to re-think our way of using it as a biomarker of long-term costs and survival prospects. This idea should be better highlighted in the discussion in my opinion.**

RESPONSE: We thank the reviewer for clarifying this point. Unfortunately we do not have any data on the presence of ITS. However, we have now added further discussion on what may cause the difference between our study and the ones mentioned above. We have also now discussed what low intra-individual repeatability may mean for the relationship between telomere length and long-term survival - lines 364-380:
RESPONSE: We thank the reviewer for clarifying this point. We have addressed the issue of ITS in a previous paper (Barrett et al. 2012; J. Avian. Biol.) - essentially we are reasonably confident that they are not an issue in our dataset, because the intra-individual changes in telomere length are too great. However, we have now added further discussion on what may cause the difference between our study and the ones mentioned above. We have also now discussed what low intra-individual repeatability may mean for the relationship between telomere length and long-term survival - lines 364-380:

\begin{adjustwidth}{2cm}{2cm}

\textit{
The finding that increases in telomere length may be sporadic and overlaid on an overall pattern of shortening with age is an important point when assessing the occurrence of telomere lengthening. Previously described approaches to distinguish telomere elongation from measurement error, based on assumptions about follow-up time between measurements (Steenstrup et al. 2013), or based on measuring variance among measurements (Simons, Stulp, and Nakagawa 2014), assume that telomere elongation within individuals is consistent over time. Our data, and that of others (Fairlie et al. 2016; Hoelzl, Cornils, et al. 2016; Hoelzl, Smith, et al. 2016) suggest that this is not the case. Such inconsistent changes in telomere length over lifespans could occur due to changes in the cellular composition of of the blood within individual samples, or due to the actual elongation of telomeres (Blackburn et al. 1989). Determining the mechanism of these changes is essential for how we view telomeres as biomarkers of costs. For example, if telomeres can be lengthened in response to improvements in environmental conditions, this would suggest that they reflect short- to medium-term costs, rather than the cumulative costs that an individual has faced over its lifespan (Bateson 2016). It is clear that telomeres can be a marker of long-term as well as short-term costs, as telomere length has been associated with both survival and lifespan in wild populations (Barrett et al. 2013; Stier et al. 2015), but we do not yet know how biologically meaningful within-individual fluctuations in telomere length are. New research is therefore required to determine when and why telomere length increases within individuals, so that biologically informed hypotheses about the nature of telomeres as biomarkers in wild populations.
The finding that increases in telomere length may be sporadic and overlaid on an overall pattern of shortening with age is an important point when assessing the occurrence of telomere lengthening. Previously described approaches to distinguish telomere elongation from measurement error, based on assumptions about follow-up time between measurements (Steenstrup et al. 2013), or based on measuring variance among measurements (Simons, Stulp, and Nakagawa 2014), assume that telomere elongation within individuals is consistent over time. Our data, and that of others (Fairlie et al. 2016; Hoelzl, Cornils, et al. 2016; Hoelzl, Smith, et al. 2016) suggest that this is not the case. Such inconsistent changes in telomere length over lifespans could occur due to changes in the cellular composition of of the blood within individual samples, due to variation in the presence of interstitial telomeric sequence, or due to the actual elongation of telomeres (Blackburn et al. 1989). Determining the mechanism of these changes is essential for how we view telomeres as biomarkers of costs. For example, if telomeres can be lengthened in response to improvements in environmental conditions, this would suggest that they reflect short- to medium-term costs, rather than the cumulative costs that an individual has faced over its lifespan (Bateson 2016). It is clear that telomeres can be a marker of long-term as well as short-term costs, as telomere length has been associated with both survival and lifespan in wild populations (Barrett et al. 2013; Stier et al. 2015), but we do not yet know how biologically meaningful within-individual fluctuations in telomere length are. New research is therefore required to determine when and why telomere length increases within individuals, so that biologically informed hypotheses about the nature of telomeres as biomarkers in wild populations.
}

\end{adjustwidth}
Expand Down
Binary file removed Manuscript/Response_to_reviewers_2.docx
Binary file not shown.
Binary file modified Manuscript/Response_to_reviewers_2.pdf
Binary file not shown.
Loading

0 comments on commit 182c06c

Please sign in to comment.